Dit is de HTML-versie van het bestand https://studenttheses.uu.nl/handle/20.500.12932/38371.
Google maakt automatisch een HTML-versie van documenten bij het indexeren van het web.
The relation between daily activity and cognitive functioning in nursing home residents with dementia
(Go: >> BACK << -|- >> HOME <<)

Page 1
The relation between daily activity and cognitive functioning in nursing
home residents with dementia
Master thesis Neuropsychology
Utrecht University
Name: Amber Heegers
Studentnumber: 5845718
Supervisors:
Dr. Irene Huenges Wajer
MSc. Angela Prins
Dr. Carlijn van den Boomen

Page 2
Abstract
Physical activity can have a positive effect on our physical health, mental health and on our
brain and cognitive functioning. There are many intervention studies that examined the
relation between physical activity and cognitive functioning in patients with dementia,
however these studies showed mixed results. Therefore, it is not clear which therapy will be
effective and if therapies can be used for specific groups of patients. To explore which
specific group(s) of patients profit most from physical therapies, this study examines the
relation between daily physical activity and cognitive functioning in nursing home residents
with dementia, while controlling for depression, level of education, dementia severity and
institutionalization time for different groups. These groups are based on walking ability (1.
patients who are able to walk, with and without support, 2. patients who are not able to walk)
and cognitive impairment (1. patients with severe cognitive impairment, 2. patients with mild
cognitive impairment). Data of sixty-eight participants was used in this study, which was
collected within two weeks, by means of cognitive tests (by means of the SIB-NL-Q),
interviews and Actiwatches. The results of this study showed no relation between daily
physical activity and cognitive functioning, with no differences for the different groups.
However, there is found a relation between walking ability and cognitive functioning. While
controlling for depression, level of education, dementia severity and institutionalization time,
this first explorative study, to the relation of, in particular daily, physical activity and
cognitive functioning may provide some handles for further research.

Page 3
Introduction
Physical activity can positively affect our physical health since it can reduce cardiovascular
disease (Sofi et al., 2007) and stress (Sofi et al., 2010). Moreover, it has a positive influence
on our cognitive functioning (Blondell, Hamersley-Mather, & Veerman, 2014), also in
patients with neurodegenerative diseases as dementia (Groot et al., 2016). Dementia is defined
as a neurodegenerative disease in which at least two cognitive domains decline from previous
functioning, wherein the patient suffers from these declines in daily life (Blondell, et al.,
2014). Many interventions including physical activity are used in patients with dementia to
improve cognitive functioning (Harris & Johnson, 2017; Park, & Cohen, 2019). Which
interventions have most effect and which patients benefit most from these interventions is
however still unclear (Park & Cohen, 2019). Furthermore, as far as known, the amount of
daily physical activity that is most effective, is not measured yet. Studying daily physical
activity, in nursing home residents with dementia, might reveal insight in what degree of
activity is beneficial for cognitive functioning and might also help to find the right patient
groups to stimulate with specific interventions.
Physical activity is defined as moving the skeletal muscles which is resulting in
expenditure of energy (Blondell et al., 2014). Different studies showed that physical activity
can have a positive effect on cognition and also can help to develop the brain. For example,
Leisman, Moustafa and Shafir (2016) argue that the development of cognitive and motor
processes are related in the brain and therefore increased physical activity will lead to a better
cognitive functioning. Physical activity contributes to neuroplasticity and helps to develop
new networks (Leisman et al., 2016; McDonnel, Buckley, Opie, Ridding, & Semmler, 2013;
Voelcker-Rehage & Niemann, 2013). This effect is independent of age and therefore it is also
seen in elderly (Voelcker-Rehage & Niemann, 2013). Since neuroplasticity underlies the
strength and the number of the connections between brain regions (Leisman et al., 2016;
Voelcker-Rehage & Niemann, 2013) and neuroplasticity is the underlying mechanism to
improve cognitive skills as learning and memory (Leisman, 2011; Leisman et al., 2016), it
seems reasonable that physical activity contributes to better cognitive functioning. For
example, physical activity might lead to a growth of the hippocampus, which might enhance
memory functions (Voelcker-Rehage & Niemann, 2013). The study of Colcombe and Kramer
(2003) showed a positive effect of physical activity on all cognitive domains, especially in
executive functioning in healthy older adults. Other studies showed improvements of
declarative memory, motor-skill coordination (McDonnel et al., 2013) and attention (Hillman,
Buck, Themanson, Pontifex, & Castelli, 2009). However, due to inconclusive results more

Page 4
research is needed, in particular more research to the relation with age related neurological
diseases as dementia and specific cognitive domains. (Prakash, Voss, Erickson, & Kramer,
2015). Moreover, the effect of physical activity can also be observed in the amount of brain
atrophy. Research of Gow et al., (2012) showed, while performing a MRI scan on older adults
at the age of 70, that physical activity is correlated with less brain atrophy three years later.
Brain atrophy is related to loss of neurons, and therefore brain volume, and one of the
biomarkers of dementia (Jack et al., 2013). This loss of brain volume is negatively correlated
to scores on the Mini Mental State Examination (Fox, Scahill, Crum, & Rossor, 1999), a
worldwide used screening instrument of dementia. More physical activity could lead to less
loss of brain volume over a longer time and therefore enhances cognitive functioning. The
study of Hamer and Chida (2009) confirmed the idea of physical activity leading to a better
cognitive functioning and even showed that less physical activity could lead to a higher risk of
neurodegenerative diseases as dementia.
Currently, a lot of physical activity therapies, such as walking interventions (Harris &
Johnson, 2017), chair yoga (Litchke, Hodges, & Reardon, 2012), strength exercises and
aerobic dance (Heyn, Abreu, & Ottenbacher, 2004), are investigated in patients with
dementia. Previous studies which have examined the effects of these therapies on cognitive
functioning in dementia have reported mixed results (Park & Cohen, 2019). Several meta-
analysis (Colcombe & Kramer, 2003; Groot, et al., 2016) have suggested that in particular
aerobic exercises show the best results. Aerobic exercises include walking (with and without
rollator) (Eggermont, Swaab, Hol, & Scherder, 2009). However, not all dementia patients are
able to perform aerobic exercises as a result of impaired motor functions. The study of
Yágüez, Shaw, Morris and Matthews (2011) has shown that non-aerobic exercises also can
improve cognitive functions in dementia patients, especially on the sustained attention, visual
information processing and working memory domains. However, the study of Miu, Szeto and
Mak (2008) found that aerobic exercises did improve the physical movement but not
cognition. These results are shared by the study of Eggermont et al. (2009) in which walking
interventions were studied. No effect of these walking interventions on cognition was found
in patients with dementia. Surprisingly, differences in frequency (Groot et al., 2016), time
(Eggermont et al., 2009) and the intensity (Varela, Ayán, Cancela, & Martín, 2011) of the
exercises do not seem to influence the effects of physical activity on cognition. These mixed
results show that it is still unclear which therapies work best and if there are specific patient
groups who might benefit more from these therapies.
Remarkable is that the above mentioned studies examined structured physical

Page 5
interventions to study the influence of physical activity on cognitive functioning while the
influence of daily physical activity is, as far as known, not studied yet. In nursing homes large
differences in the residents’ activity level is observed. For example, nursing homes residents
might be physical agitated and therefore wandering is frequently observed (Cipriani, Lucetti,
Nuti, & Danti, 2014). On the contrary, many nursing home residents with dementia sit in a
chair all day which might be due to that nursing home environments might encourage
physical inactivity (Tappen, Roach, Buchner, Barry, & Edelstein, 1997). Also, patients with a
high risk of falling are often placed in a wheelchair for precaution. Taken this together, the
time spent in a nursing home might influence the relation between physical activity and
cognitive functioning. Moreover, this inactivity might be caused by depression (Barlow &
Durand, 2015) which is present in up to 20% to 37% of the patients with dementia (Kuring,
Mathias, & Ward, 2018). Since there is an enormous observed variability in daily physical
activity in nursing home residents with dementia and physical activity is associated with
better cognitive functioning (Blondell et al., 2014; Hamer & Chida, 2009), it is likely that
cognitive functioning differs in nursing home residents with dementia due to daily physical
activity.
Besides the effect of depression on physical activity, depression can also affect
cognitive functioning. Depression can lead to cognitive dysfunction and make individuals
score worse on different cognitive domains (Koenig et al., 2015) and can cause a progression
of dementia in patients who are mildly cognitive impaired (Mourao, Mansur, Malloy-Diniz,
Costa, & Diniz, 2015). Another factor that can influence the cognitive functioning is the level
of premorbid education. Older adults with higher education performed better on cognitive
tasks than older adults that had lower education (van Hooren et al., 2007). Furthermore, in
contrast to patients with lower educational levels, higher education leads to higher cognitive
reserve which can help dementia patients function better for a longer time (Stern, 2012).
Therefore, in this study we investigate the relation between physical activity in daily
life and cognitive functioning in nursing home residents with dementia, while adjusting for
depression, premorbid education level, dementia severity and institutionalization time. The
aim of the current study is to find out more about the relation between daily physical activity
and cognitive functioning and to examine if specific groups of patients might benefit more
from this daily physical activity. In this way, physical therapies could be adapted and
introduced to patients that might truly benefit from these therapies. If a positive relation
between daily life activity and cognition is found it is interesting to study if this relation can
also be found for the different cognitive domains. Besides, there will be explored if there is a

Page 6
difference in effect of daily life activity on cognition for patients with different stages of
cognitive impairment. This information can tell even more about if specific groups of patients
with dementia will benefit from more physical activity. For example, when the results show
that patients with severe cognitive impairment do not benefit from physical activity, they do
not have to be overloaded with physical therapy to increase the cognitive functioning. In the
current study, it is first hypothesized that, based on previous studies that examined the effect
of physical activity on cognitive functioning (Blondell et al., 2014; Hamer and Chida 2009;
Sofi et al., 2010), more physical activity is related to better overall cognitive functioning. It is
expected that this relation between daily physical activity still exists while controlling for the
effects of depression, level of education, dementia severity and institutionalization time on
cognitive functioning. This is expected for the first group; patients who are able to walk,
including walking with support, and the second group; patients who are not able to walk. Both
groups are able to perform a kind of physical activity, aerobic or non-aerobic, which both
have shown to be effective (Groot et al., 2016; Yágüez et al., 2011). However, a stronger
effect of daily physical activity on overall cognitive functioning is expected for patients who
are able to walk (with and without support), compared with patients who are not able to walk.
This is expected since walking, with rollator, is an aerobic exercise (Eggermont et al., 2009)
which seems to have the best effects on cognitive functioning (Colcombe & Kramer, 2003;
Groot, et al., 2016). Second, the positive relation between daily physical activity and
cognitive functioning as described in the first hypothesis, is expected for all separate cognitive
domains (Colcombe & Kramer, 2003; Eggermont et al., 2009; Groot, et al., 2016; Yágüez et
al., 2011). Third, it is hypothesized that there will be a different relation of daily activity to
cognitive functioning for the different stages of cognitive impairment. It is expected that this
relation between daily physical activity still exists while controlling for the effects of
depression, level of education, dementia severity and institutionalization time on cognitive
functioning. Which group will benefit most is not to say yet since this is, as far as known, not
investigated before. Still a difference between the groups is expected since the course of
dementia shows differences in brain atrophy (Jack et al., 2013). A more affected brain might
benefit more from physical activity and the neuroplasticity whereby bigger steps could be
made. On the other hand, an intact brain might benefit more from physical activity since it is
able to learn more. (Moore, Sandman, McGrady, & Kesslak, 2010; Zarit, Zarit, & Reever,
1982).

Page 7
Methods
Participants
Current study included 97 participants. The participant group contained residents who used to
live at four departments of nursing home ‘Atlant’ at Apeldoorn during the period of
November 2018 to January 2020. In total 144 participants were invited to participate in the
study, 44 contact persons refused participation.
The inclusion criteria was diagnosis of dementia according to ICD-10 and DSM-IV
criteria. Exclusion criteria for this study were; a life expectation of less than four weeks
according to a doctor and residents or contact persons that did not give verbal and written
consent. Participants were (partly) excluded from the research when the participants or the
contact persons reported discomfort during the research process. In total 3 participants were
excluded from the research due to discomfort or death. The data collection is approved by the
Medical Ethics Committee of the ‘Vrije Universiteit van Amsterdam’ (VUmc) and by the
Science Committee of ‘Atlant’.
Measurement instruments
Cognitive functioning
To measure the cognitive abilities of the participants the Mini Mental State Examination
(MMSE) (Folstein et al, 1975) and a shortened Dutch version of the Severe Impairment
Battery (SIB-NL-Q) (de Jonghe, Wetzels, Mulders, Zuidema, & Koopmans, 2009) were used.
The MMSE measures general cognitive functioning, with 20 questions. The items contain
questions as “In which country are we?’ but also instructions as ‘Do you want to think of a
sentence and write it down on this paper?’ Most answers are scored by means of a two point
scale with options ‘Correct’ and ‘Incorrect’. The MMSE can be scored by counting up all
points that are obtained (minimum score = 0, maximum score = 30). This scale has a high
inter-rater reliability kw = 0.97 and the validity is dependent of premorbid education (Galea &
Woodward, 2005). The stage of cognitive impairment was determined by the performance on
the MMSE, by using cut-off scores of ≤ 17 which corresponds to severe cognitive impaired
and ≥ 18 which corresponds to mild cognitive impaired to normal. (O’ Connor et al., 1889).
The SIB(-NL-Q) is developed for patients with severe dementia that cannot complete
neuropsychological tests anymore. It contains nine different subscales (cognitive domains);
social interaction, memory, orientation, language, attention, praxis, visuospatial ability,
construction and orienting to name. The SIB-NL-Q has 26 items and the items contain
questions about the different domains as ‘Can you read this card and do as it says?’. Most

Page 8
items are scored on a 3 point scale with 4 answer options; 2 points ‘Spontaneous correct’, 1
point ‘Correct after encouragement’, 0 points ‘Not correct’, 0 points ‘No answer’. The total
score can be calculated by counting up all the scores of the sub-scales of the SIB (minimum
score = 0, maximum score = 50). The short version of the SIB has a high construct validity
(de Jonghe et al., 2009). The SIB-NL-Q and MMSE have a high correlation of r = 0.97 which
means they measure the same construct (Qazi, et al. 2005).
Daily activity
The daily activity of the participants was measured with wrist worn Actiwatches of type AW4
(Actiwatch, Cambridge Neurotechnology, Cambridge, UK). The Actiwatches were set at one
epoch per minute and these were calibrated each half year. Actiwatches were chosen to
measure the daily activity in the participants because it can measure the activity for 24 hours a
day and are less invasive in comparison with measurement methods that measure brain
activity as electroencephalography (EEG). The study of Gironda, Lloyd, Clark and Walker
(2007) measured the interunit reliability of Actiwatches, which means the reliability of the
Actiwatch when it is at the same body site. Actiwatches, worn at the wrist, have a interunit
reliability of r = .56 compared to the waist and r = .58 compared to the ankle (Gironda et al.,
2007). The construct validity of the Actiwatches is high which means that the Actiwatch is a
good measure for different kind of movements. During walking an Actiwatch worn on the
wrist correlates high to the score as an Actiwatch worn on the ankle (Gironda et al., 2007).
Since the participants in our study will wear the Actiwatch on the wrist it will be a reliable
and valid measure for daily physical activity.
The 10 consecutive most active hours (M10) will be used to represent the degree of
daily physical activity of the participants. M10 contains information about how regular the
activity was and how active the participant was during these 10 hours. (Burns, Allen,
Tomenson, Duignan, & Byrne, 2009). It measures intensity and frequency of physical activity
during the 10 most active hours of the patient within 24 hours over within eight days.
Therefore, this variable of activity fits as a measure of daily activity because it gives a mean
of the activity pattern during their 10 most active hours of the day.
Ability to walk
Care workers were asked about the ability of the participants to walk using three answer
possibilities; able to walk, able to walk with support (unilateral and bilateral) and not able to
walk.

Page 9
Control variables
Depression
The degree of depression was measured with the Dutch version of the Cornell Scale for
Depression in Dementia (Dröes, 1993) which was administered with care workers. This scale
is a screening for depression in dementia patients and has five subscales which include; mood
related characteristics, behavioural disorders, physical characteristics, cyclic features,
disorders in thoughts. The Cornell has 19 items which contain questions as ‘Do you observe
sadness in the patient?’. All items are scored by means of four answer options; 2 points
Severe’, 1 point ‘Lightly or varying’ 0 points ‘Absent’, 0 points, ‘Not to asses’. The total
score of the Cornell is calculated by counting up all the points that are obtained (minimum
score = 0, maximum score = 38). The inter-rater reliability of the Cornell is kw = 0.67 which is
high and the validity of measuring depression in nursing homes is rated as good, with a score
of rs = 0.80. (Alexopoulos, Abrams, Young, and Shamoian, 1988).
Dementia severity
The dementia severity was measured with the Global Deterioration Scale (GDS), which
divides dementia in seven stages from ‘No cognitive disorder (normal adult)’, ‘Very mild
cognitive disorder’, ‘Mild cognitive decline’, ‘Moderate cognitive decline’, ‘Moderate severe
cognitive decline’, ‘Severe cognitive decline’ to ‘Very severe cognitive decline (last stage
Alzheimer dementia)’ (Reisberg, Ferris, de Leon, & Crook, 1982). The GDS has a high inter-
rater reliability of kw = .82 to kw = .92 (Eisdorfer et al., 1993).
Level of education
To measure the premorbid level of cognitive capacity of the participants, the level of
education was measured with the Dutch coding system of Verhage (1964), which divides the
level of education in seven different levels from ‘Not finished Primary school’, ‘Finished
primary school’, ‘Finished primary school and less than two years of low level secondary
education’, ‘Finished low level secondary education’, ‘Finished average level of secondary
education’, ‘Finished high level of secondary education’ to ‘Completed university’.
Institutionalization time
The institutionalization time is defined as the time since the participants were administered to
the current psychogeriatric ward of Atlant. This time was rounded down to years.

Page 10
Design
The data that was used in the current study, is part of the study Het effect van verrijkte
omgeving op cognitie en kwaliteit van leven van patiënten met dementie’ from MSc. Angela
Prins and prof. dr. E. J. A. Scherder. This is a cohort study with a prospective nature, since the
participants are followed for at least three measurement moments while receiving an
intervention. For the current study only data from the baseline measurements, collected
between from November 2018 and January 2020, was used. This current study can be
described as a qualitative case report study. To enlarge the reliability of the study, all
researchers, master students in neuropsychology, were trained to administer the tests in the
same way.
Procedure
To include the participants in the study the first contact person of the resident received an
information letter and informed consent forms. When the inclusion was not completed in two
weeks, an independent secretary who was not directly involved in the research, called the
contact person to remind about the inclusion and provided more information when necessary.
After inclusion, the data of each participant was collected within two weeks. This data
collection contained data from three parts; cognition, physical activity and mood. The SIB-
NL-Q, the MMSE and M10 were administered with the participants and the Cornell and GDS
were administered by the first responsible care worker. There was not a thigh structure in
which the three sorts of data were collected. Prior to the administration of the SIB-NL-Q and
the MMSE, the researchers approached care workers and asked them if there were any
properties of the participant that had to be taken into account, as hearing, sight or behaviour.
This information was used during the tests to approach the participant in the best way.
Thereafter the participants themselves were approached to participate with the tests. The test
were taken in a quiet place as the bedroom of the participant, so the participant was not
distracted by the surrounding. The administration of the SIB-NL-Q took about 15 to 20
minutes and thereafter the MMSE was administered which took about 15 to 20 minutes. To
measure the daily activity the Actiwatch was putted on the dominant hand of the participants
for eight days and diaries were attached to the bathroom door of the participant at the first
day. The Actiwatches were only allowed to take off while showering, the care workers needed
to report the time the Actiwatches went off and on again on the diaries. The exact time that
the Actiwatch was putted on and taken off was noted. If the participant stated that they did not
wanted to wear the Actiwatch anymore or discomfort was observed the Actiwatch was taken

Page 11
of at all times. To measure the ability to walk, degree of depression and dementia severity
with the Cornell and GDS the first responsible care worker were interviewed about their
observations of the participant in the last two weeks prior to the interview. For this
questionnaire the researchers had to pay attention that the concepts of questions were
understood and answered correctly by the care workers. This interview took about 15 to 20
minutes. The procedure for administering the Cornell has changed during the data collection.
At the start of the study, answers on the questions were based on changes that were seen from
earlier to the last two weeks. With this approach, there is a chance that depression symptoms
that exist for longer than two weeks will be missed. Therefore, the procedure was changed
since July 2019. Answers were now based on observations from the last two weeks. After all
data was collected and stored in Castor (Castor EDC, 2019) the data of the participants was
separated into two different groups based on the ability to walk; (1) able to walk, which
includes walking with support, and (2) not able to walk and the data was analysed.
Data processing steps and data-analyses
Power analysis
According to a power analysis (Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner, & Lang, 2009) 55 participants are
needed (power .80, α = .05, F2 = .15) to perform multiple regression with five predictors.
Actigraphy
The data that was collected with the Actiwatches was read out using a software computer
program Sleep Analysis version 7 (Actiwatch, Cambridge Neurotechnology, Cambridge,
UK). The actogram and the diaries were examined to check if the Actiwatches were worn
continuously, the times the Actiwatches went off, noted in the diaries, was correct. This was
checked by searching for the gaps in the actogram. For gaps in the actogram, from which the
cause was unclear, the possible cause was checked by the care workers. For example, these
gaps could be explained by a showering moment, in which the Actiwatch was off, that was
not written down in the diaries. Gaps that were correctly noted in the diaries and gaps that
were seen in the actogram that lasted longer than an hour were noted un a Microsoft Excel
template developed by Van Someren (1999). This template was able to cut out the gaps from
the data and ran multiple analyses with the data.
Statistical Analyses
The statistical analyses were performed using SPSS Statistics version 25 (IBM Corp., 2017).
The assumptions for ‘independence’, ‘normality’, ‘homogeneity of regression slopes’,
‘linearity’ and ‘homogeneity of variance’ were checked before the main analyses were

Page 12
performed. Furthermore, a visual data inspection was performed over the data, which included
means, minima and maxima.
To test the first hypothesis, a hierarchical multiple regression analysis for the two
groups, able to walk and not able to walk, with dependent variable ‘SIB-NL-Q total score’
was performed. In the first box the predictors ‘Cornell’, ‘Education level’, ‘GDS’ and
‘institutionalization time’ were added to the model and in the second box the independent
variable ‘M10’ was added to the model. The main outcome showed how the relation between
daily physical activity and cognitive functioning existed and differed for the two groups.
When the results showed a significant positive relation between daily physical activity and
cognitive functioning, the second hypothesis was tested with several hierarchical multiple
regression analysis for the two groups (able to walk and not able to walk), with dependent
variable ‘SIB-NL-Q sub-scores’ were performed. In the first box the predictors ‘Cornell’,
‘education level’, ‘GDS’ and ‘institutionalization time’ were added to the model and in the
second box the independent variable ‘M10’ was added to the model. In addition, to test the
third hypothesis, extra analysis were performed to show the relation between daily activity
and cognition for different stages of dementia. First, an ANOVA was performed to see if the
SIB-NL-Q scores differed between the four groups. Then, a hierarchical multiple regression
analysis for the four groups, able to walk and mild cognitive impaired, able to walk and
severe cognitive impaired, not able to walk and mild cognitive impaired and not able to walk
and severe cognitive impaired, the same as the first analysis, was performed. Each of the
analyses was performed with a p value of .05.

Page 13
Results
Participants
In this study, 29 participants (29.9% of the total sample) were excluded from the statistical
analyses due to missing data in the main variables (SIB total score and M10). 14% of the total
data set was missing. Data of the remaining 68 participants was included in the analyses. The
group of participants that was excluded did not differ from the included group with respect to
age, gender, walking ability, type of dementia, level of education, SIB-NL-Q scores, MMSE
scores and Cornell scores. (all p > 0.5). Information about the participant group included into
the analyses is presented in table 1.
Table 1
Demographic characteristics of sample included in the analyses
Variables
Total (n = 68)
Able to walk (n = 55)
Not able to walk (n =13)
Age, Mean (SD)
85.52 (7.01)
85,35 (6.93)
86,23 (7.60)
Gender (male/female)
Alzheimer disease
Vascular dementia
27/41 (40%/60%)
21 (30.9%)
10 (14.7%)
24/31 (44%/56%)
15 (27.3%)
9 (16.4%)
3/10 (23%/77%)
6 (46.2%)
1 (7.7%)
Frontotemporal disease
Lewybody disease
2 (2.9%)
3 (4.4 %)
2 (3.6%)
3 (5.5%)
0 (0%)
0 (0%)
Combined dementia
11 (16.2%)
9 16.4%)
2 (15.4%)
Dementia syndrome
21 (30.9 %)
17 (30.9%)
4 (30.8%)
MCI
5 (7.4%)
5 (9.1%)
0 (0%)
SCI
63 (92.6%)
50 (90.9%)
13 (100%)
Vascular disease
SIB-NL-Q, Mean (SD)
MMSE, Mean (SD)
Cornell, Mean (SD)
55 (80.9%)
33.06 (12.16)
8.72 (5.73)
7.34 (5.54)
46 (83.6%)
34.82 (10.92)
9.15 (5.87)
6.50 (5.35)
9 (69.2%)
25.62 (14.66)
6.92 (5.31)
10.69 (5.02)
Note. SCI = Severe cognitive impaired, MCI = Mild cognitive impaired.
Assumptions and data inspection
The assumption of normality was violated for variables SIB total score (p ≤ .001), MMSE (p
≤ .05), Cornell (p ≤. 001). Therefore, a Van der Waerden transformation (van der Waerden,
1952) was performed. All the other assumptions of the hierarchical regression analysis
(independence, homogeneity of regression slopes, linearity and homogeneity of variance)
were not violated.
Except from the missing data, no particularities were found from the visual data

Page 14
inspection. Correlational analysis yielded a not significant correlation between the Cornell
score and SIB total score and between the level of education and the SIB total score (see table
2). Therefore, it was decided that these variables were not included as control variables in the
main analyses.
Table 2
Correlations between the different variables.
1 2 3
4
5
6
7
8
1. SIBtotal
- .05
-.30*
.84*
-.22
-.52*
-.09
-.31*
2. M10
-
-.05
-.01
.15
-.07
.16
-.17
3. Ability to walk
-
-.16
.32*
.04
-.14 .09
4. MMSE
-
-.19
-.60*
-.09
-.21
5. Cornell
-
-.02
-.00 -.04
6. GDS
-
.10
.15
7. Level of education
-
-.03
8. Institutionalization
time
-
*p ≤ .05

Page 15
Hypothesis 1
To test the first hypothesis, if there is a positive relation between M10 and the SIB total score
when controlling for GDS and institutionalization time for both of the two groups but stronger
for the walking group, a hierarchical multiple regression was performed. Within the able to
walk group block one of the hierarchical multiple regression, GDS score and
institutionalization time accounted for an insignificant proportion of the variance in the SIB
total score. In the second block M10 was added to the regression. Independent of the GDS
score and institutionalization time, M10 explained no significant proportion of the variance in
the SIB total score. The total model including GDS, institutionalization time and M10
explained no significant proportion in SIB total score. More information about the R values, F
values and the unstandardized and standardized regression coefficients are reported in table 3.
Within the not able to walk group, block one of the hierarchical multiple regression,
GDS score and institutionalization time, did not account for a significant proportion of
variance in SIB total score. In the second block M10 was added to the regression. Independent
of GDS and institutionalization time, M10 did not explain a significant proportion of variance
in SIB total score. The total model including GSD, institutionalization time and M10
explained no significant proportion in SIB total score. More information about the R values, F
values and the unstandardized and standardized regression coefficients are reported in table 3.
Hypothesis 2
To test the second hypothesis, participants in both groups that show high levels of physical
activity will score better on all cognitive domains when controlling for the effects of dementia
severity and institutionalization time on cognitive functioning, with higher effects for the
patients who are able to walk, there had to be a significant relation between the SIB and M10.
Since this relation was not significant (see table 3), the second hypothesis was not tested.
Therefore no further statements can be made about the relation between the subscale scores of
the SIB and M10 controlled for GDS and institutionalization time.
Hypothesis 3
To test the third hypothesis, if there will be a different relation of daily activity to cognitive
functioning for the different stages of cognitive impairment (mild cognitive impairment and
severe cognitive impairment) while controlling for dementia severity and institutionalization
time, a hierarchical multiple regression was performed. This was performed for the four
groups, able to walk and severe cognitive impaired (n = 50), able to walk and mild cognitive
impaired (n = 5), not able to walk and mild cognitive impaired (n = 0) and not able to walk

Page 16
and severe cognitive impaired (n = 13). None of the participants accounted to the criteria of
the group not able to walk and mild cognitive impaired, therefore the analysis will be
performed with only three groups.
First, an ANOVA was performed to check if all the groups the SIB total score indeed
differed between the three groups. The ANOVA was statistically significant, indicating that
the SIB total score did differ between the groups, F (2, 65) = 11.17, p ≤. 001, 2 = .26. Post
hoc analyses with Tukey’s HSD revealed that the SIB total score for participants who are able
to walk with severe cognitive impairments (M = .01, SD = .81) was significantly lower than
participants who are able to walk with mild cognitive impairments (M = 1.71, SD = .43). In
addition, the SIB total score for participants who are not able to walk with severe cognitive
impairments (M = -.59, SD = 1.07) is significantly lower than the SIB total score for
participants who are able to walk with mild cognitive impairments (M = .01, SD = .81).
However, the SIB total score did not significantly differ for participants who are able to walk
with severe cognitive impairments (M = .01, SD = .81) and for participants who are not able
to walk with severe cognitive impairments (M = .01, SD = .81).
Then, the hierarchical multiple regression was performed. Within the first group, able
to walk and severe cognitive impaired, block one of the hierarchical multiple regression, GDS
score and institutionalization time, accounted for a significant 17.9% of the variance, (p ≤.
05). In the second block M10 was added to the regression. Independent of GDS and
institutionalization time, M10 did not explain a significant proportion of variance in SIB total
score. The total model including GSD, institutionalization time and M10 explained 18.4% of
the variance in SIB total score (p ≤ .05). More information about the R values, F values and
the unstandardized and standardized regression coefficients are reported in table 4.
Within the second group, able to walk and mild cognitive impaired, block one of the
hierarchical multiple regression, GDS score and institutionalization time, did not explain a
significant proportion of variance in SIB total score. In the second block M10 was added to
the regression. Independent of GDS and institutionalization time, M10 did not explain a
significant proportion of variance in SIB total score. The total model including GDS, and
institutionalization time and M10 did not explain a significant proportion of variance in SIB
total score. More information about the R values, F values and the unstandardized and
standardized regression coefficients are reported in table 4.
Within the third group, not able to walk and severe cognitive impaired, block one of
the hierarchical multiple regression, GDS score and institutionalization time, did explain a
significant 52.9% the variance in SIB total score (p ≤ .05). In the second block M10 was

Page 17
added to the regression. Independent of GDS and institutionalization time, M10 did not
explain a significant proportion of variance in SIB total score. The total model including
GDS, institutionalization time and M10 did not explain a significant proportion of variance in
SIB total score. More information about the R values, F values and the unstandardized and
standardized regression coefficients are reported in table 4.
Table 3
Unstandardized (B) and Standardized (β) regression coefficients for each variable on each
step of a hierarchical multiple regression predicting SIB total score, divided by ability to
walk.
Variable
B [95% CI]
β
R2
ΔR2
F
Able to walk
(n = 55)
Block 1
GDS
Uptake
Block 2
GDS
Time
M10
Total
Not able to walk
(n = 13)
Block 1
GDS
Time
Block 2
GDS
Time
M10
Total
0.69 [-0,12, 1,50]
-0.10 [-0.22, 0,02]
0.68 [-0.14, 1.51]
-0.10 [-0.22, 0.03]
0.00 [-0.02, 0.03]
0.80 [-0.90, 2.49]
-0.17 [-0.43, 0.06]
0.73 [-1,19, 2,65]
-0.20 [-0.48, 0.9]
-0.01 [-0.07, 0.09]
0.23
-0.22
0.22n
-0.22
0.04
0.28
-0.45
0.26
-0.47
-0.07
.11
.11
.29
.29
.11
.00
.29
.00
3.08
.11
2.05
2.00
.05
1.22

Page 18
Note. CI = Confidence interval, GDS = Global Deterioration Scale dementia severity, Time =
institutionalization time in years, M10 = Daily physical activity.
*p ≤ .05
Table 4
Unstandardized (B) and Standardized (β) regression coefficients for each variable on each
step of a hierarchical multiple regression predicting SIB total score, divided by the four
groups; Able to walk and severe cognitive impaired (SCI), Able to walk and mild cognitive
impaired (MCI), Not able to walk and severe cognitive impaired, Not able to walk and mild
cognitive impaired.
Variable
B [95% CI]
β
R2
ΔR2 F
Able to walk,
SCI (n = 48)
Block 1
GDS
Time
Block 2
GDS
Time
M10
Total
Able to walk,
MCI (n = 4)
Block 1
GDS
-0.43 [-0.73, -0.14]
-0.06 [-0.18, 0.06]
-0.45 [-0.75, -0.15]
-0.06 [-0.18, 0.06]
-0.01 [-0.03, 0,02]
-0.43 [-0.43, -0.43]
-0.40
-0.14
-0.41
-0.14
-0.07
-1.00
.18
18.4
1.00
.18
.01
1.00
4.91*
.28
3.32*

Page 19
Note. CI = Confidence interval, GDS = Global Deterioration Scale dementia severity, Time =
institutionalization time in years, M10 = Daily physical activity, SCI = Severe cognitive
impaired, MCI = Mild cognitive impaired.
*p ≤ .05
Time
Block 2
GDS
Time
M10
Total
Not able to walk,
SCI (n = 13)
Block 1
GDS
Time
Block 2
GDS
Time
M10
Total
0.00 [-0.00, 0.00]
-0.43 [-0.43, -0.43]
0.00 [0.00, 0.00]
0.00 [0.00, 0.00]
-0.63 [-1.17, -0.09]
-0.10 [-0.31, 0.12]
-0.63 [-1.23, 0.03]
-0.10 [-0.35, 0.15]
0.00 [-0.05, 0.05]
0.00
-1,00
0.00
0.00
-0.61
-0.24
-0.61
-0.24
0.01
1.00
.53
.53
.00
.53
.00
5.61*
3.74
.00
3.37

Page 20
Discussion
In this study, the relation between daily physical activity and cognitive functioning in nursing
home residents with dementia, with different walking abilities, was examined while
controlling for depression, education level, dementia severity and institutionalization time. It
was hypothesized that a higher daily physical activity would lead to a better cognitive
functioning, for both patients who were able to walk and patients who were not able to walk,
with a greater effect for patients who were able to walk. Moreover, it was hypothesized that if
this relation between daily physical activity and cognitive functioning was found, it would
have an influence on all cognitive domains and there would be a difference in the relation
between daily physical activity and cognitive functioning for patients with different stages of
cognitive impairment. The results of this study showed, contrary to the expectations, that
higher daily physical activity was not associated with a higher level of cognitive functioning
in both patients who were able to walk and patients who were not able to walk. However,
independent of the daily physical activity, patients who were able to walk had a better level of
cognitive functioning than the patients who were not able to walk anymore. Since no relation
was found between daily physical activity and cognitive functioning, no further statements
were made about the relation between daily physical activity and the different cognitive
domains. Moreover, when the participants were divided based on their cognitive impairment,
no relation between daily physical activity and cognition was found either. Despite depression
and level of education are frequently reported predictors for cognitive functioning (van
Hooren et al., 2007; Koenig et al., 2015; Mourao et al., 2015; Stern, 2012), in this current
study depression and level of education did not relate to cognitive functioning.
That we did not find a relation between daily physical activity and cognitive
functioning is not in line with previous research (Colcombe & Kramer, 2003; Hillman et al.,
2009; McDonnel et al., 2013; Voelcker-Rehage & Niemann, 2013). More specifically,
Colcombe and Kramer (2003) found that physical activity has a positive effect on cognition in
general. Moreover, other studies found, contrary to the results of the current study, effects of
physical activity on specific cognitive domains (Hillman, et al., 2009 McDonnel et al., 2013;
Voelcker-Rehage & Niemann, 2013). On the other hand, the finding of the current study does
correspond with work of Eggermont et al. (2009). They did not find an effect of walking on
cognitive functioning even though walking, even while using a rollator, was categorized as an
aerobic exercise, which is the type of exercise that should have the biggest effect on cognitive
functioning (Colcombe & Kramer, 2003; Groot et al., 2016). The finding of the current study
that walking ability, independent of daily physical activity, is related with cognitive

Page 21
functioning is comparable to previous work of Kikkert, Vuillerme, van Campend, Hortobágyi,
& Lamotha, (2016), which found that walking ability might be a biomarker for cognitive
decline.
The lack of relation between depression and education level with cognitive functioning
does not correspond to the current overall literature (van Hooren et al., 2007; Koenig et al.,
2015; Mourao et al., 2015; Stern, 2012). The study of Mourao et al. (2015) found that
depression could contribute to progression of dementia in patients that are mildly cognitive
impaired. The current study did not find a relation between depression and cognitive
functioning in dementia patients. Likewise, the finding that education level had no relation
with cognitive functioning is contradictory to the results of the study from Stern (2012) who
found that higher education might lead to a better cognitive reserve and to a better cognitive
functioning than by patients with a lower education. More specific, regarding to the SIB
scores, which are in this study used as a measure of cognitive functioning, did the study of
Henskens, Nauta, Drost, Milders, & Scherder (2019) find a relation between SIB score and
mood. In that study mood was measured with the Care Dependency Scale (CDS) which
measures depression, apathy and agitation. However, the study of Wajman and Bertolucci
(2006) shows that there is no relation between level of education and SIB-score, which
corresponds to the current study.
The non-existent relation between daily physical activity and cognitive functioning
that was found can be explained in multiple ways. As stated in the introduction, the
development and networks of cognitive and motor processes are related (Leisman et al.,
2016). Since neurodegeneration is one of the biomarkers for dementia and since it can lead to
cognitive deficits (Fox et al., 2013; Jack et al., 2013), this could also lead to the loss of motor
processes and less daily physical activity. Therefore, it is possible that the differences in
cognition and daily physical activity that are seen between nursing home residents with
dementia are more due to neurodegeneration and are not caused by the differences in daily
physical activity exclusively. Another reason for the absence of the relation between daily
physical activity and cognitive functioning might be that in these patients the blood flow
during and after the physical activity was reduced (Eggermont et al., 2009). In healthy
participants, after physical activity the blood flow will rise, which can decrease the effect of
aging (Lucas et al., 2012). This might not be the case in patients with cardiovascular disease,
where the cardiac output blood flow, in the brain, is reduced during exercise (Eggermont et
al., 2009). The study of Perea et al. (2016) suggested that Alzheimer patients with a higher
cardiorespiratory fitness might have better preserved white matter integrity. In this population

Page 22
of nursing home residents with dementia, cardiovascular diseases are very common and in the
current study many participants had cardiovascular diseases (80.9%). Therefore,
cardiovascular diseases might contribute to a lessened effect of daily physical activity on
cognition as it might cause a reduced blood flow in the brain and less preserved white matter
integrity (Eggermont et al., 2009; Perea et al., 2016). Moreover, it might be possible that the
effect of (daily) physical activity in the younger years also could have influenced the
cognitive functioning that the patients show now. The study of Hakala et al. (2019) showed
that the physical activity in childhood and young adulthood had an influence of the cognitive
functioning in midlife. This effect was independent of the physical activity on other age. This
is confirmed by the study of Reas et al. (2019), which found that more physical activity in the
teenage years could lead to a better cognitive functioning in the older years. This might be
caused by building more cognitive reserve in the younger years. They also found that the
effect was stronger when the participants were physical active both at younger age as at older
age. This is also confirmed by the meta-analysis of Engeroff, Ingmann and Banzer (2018).
More longitudinal research in this area is needed.
Still, in the current study was found that patients who were able to walk had a better
cognitive performance than patients who were not able to walk. According to Kikkert et al.
(2016) this relation between walking ability and cognitive functioning might be due to age
related loss of brain volume. Contrary to the results of Kikkert et al. (2016), the results of the
current study showed that lower cognitive functioning in patients who are not able to walk is
independent of age and the dementia severity (see table 2). This might mean that this lower
cognitive function is not related with general decline associated with aging or dementia
severity of the patients. The literature also does not seem to have an explanation for this
difference between these groups. Possibly, the relation between walking ability and cognitive
functioning could still be explained by physical activity (Blondell et al., 2014; Groot et al.,
2016). However, this might be found with another measure for daily physical activity than the
M10 since the M10 does not give specific information about the type and intensity of the
movements.
No difference was found in the relation between the daily physical activity and
cognitive functioning for patients with different levels of cognitive impairment. This might be
explained by the scores on the cognitive tests. The scores on the MMSE for all participants
were in a small range. 92.7% of the participants scored 17 points or less on the MMSE. This
might mean that all participants approximately were on the same cognitive level, which is
seen as severe cognitive impaired (O’ Connor et al., 1989). An explanation could be that the

Page 23
MMSE is not as sensitive for the cognitive functioning of severely cognitive impaired
individuals as the SIB-NL-Q. The participants of this study show way more variation on the
total scores of the SIB-NL-Q (M = 33,06; SD = 12,16). This might explain why no differences
between the groups divided on the level of cognitive functioning were found.
A possible reason for the finding that depression was not related to cognitive
functioning might be explained by the research of Vinkers, Gussekloo, Stek, Westendorp and
van der Mast (2004). They found that depression might develop when cognitive decline is
present in a person and when that person is aware of the cognitive decline. Therefore,
depression might not be a risk factor but a consequence of cognitive decline. It is possible that
the dementia patients in the nursing home are (as part of their disease) not aware of their
cognitive decline (Aalten, Van Valen, Clare, Kenny, & Verhey, 2005) and therefore do not
develop a depression or depressive symptoms. The reason why level of education was not
related with cognitive functioning in the current study, might be due to the time in which
these participants grew up. The mean age of the participants is 85.5 years, which means the
time they went to school was during and after war, in a less wealthy society. It is possible that
during that time the possibilities for further education were limited. This might mean that the
education that the participants received was not presentable for their premorbid level of
cognitive capacity (Legdeur et al., 2017). Moreover, in this study cognitive functioning is
measured by the SIB total score, as mentioned before, the study of Wajman and Bertolucci
(2006) also did not find a relation between education and SIB total score. They state it might
be possible that the effect of education might disappear when dementia appears or that
differences in premorbid education no longer contribute to the level of the performance on
simple SIB tasks which is developed for severe dementia. These might be the reasons why the
education level does not correspond to cognitive functioning of the participants.
A strength of this study is that the relation between daily physical activity and
cognition including multiple possible confounding variables is extensively studied. Moreover,
as far as known this study is the first study that measures the relation between daily physical
activity and cognition. Still, the current study has some limitations which should be taken into
consideration. Since a lot of the data from the data set was missing (14% of the total data set)
many participants had to be excluded from the research (n = 29). This data was missing since
not all participants wanted or were able to cooperate with the tests. It is important that the
wellbeing of the participants was guaranteed and that patients that reported discomfort, or
from whom discomfort was observed, were excluded from the study. As well due to the big
exclusion rate this study had a small sample size. According to the Power Analysis (Faul et

Page 24
al., 2009), 55 participants were needed to perform the initial analyses. The analyses were done
with two groups which means that both groups had to exist of 55 participants. On forehand
was unknown how many participants could be included in this study, however it was known
that this amount of participants (n = 55 per group) was not achievable. Since this study was an
explorative study, it was chosen to persevere in this design. However, with a small sample
size, caution must be applied, the small sample size caused a lower power (Neuman, 2014)
which makes the statements of this study less reliable.
Moreover, during the data-collection the procedure of the Cornell depression scale was
changed. More specifically, the procedure was changed from questions about the change in
the last two week to questions about the observed behaviour. This has influenced the data,
therefore it could be argued that it would have been better to just keep one procedure.
Though, it has changed the data in a positive way since with the new procedure more
depression symptoms could recorded, this makes the data more valid. With the first procedure
depressive symptoms from patients that had these symptoms for longer than two weeks were
missed. Therefore it was chosen to continue with the change in procedure.
Furthermore, concerning measuring daily physical activity, it might have been more
accurate to analyse the hours of activity only during daytime. In healthy individuals most
physical activity is performed during daytime and night time is used for sleep. Therefore, only
analysing the hours only during daytime might be more representable for measuring, in
particular daily, physical activity. The M10 just measures the 10 most active hours in the 24
hours of a day, which also might have been in the evening or at night time for a lot of
participant. Since patients with dementia might show disturbed day and night rhythms
(Hooghiemstra, Eggermont, Scheltens, van der Flier, & Scherder 2015; Leng, Musiek, Hu,
Cappuccio, & Yaffe, 2019) it also could be argued that physical activity in the evening or at
night time is part of activity during their day. It is not known if measuring daily physical
activity during daytime only or during the day and night time will make a difference for the
relation between daily physical activity and cognitive functioning. This might be an
interesting point of focus for further research. The M10 seemed to be a very good variable for
daily physical activity since these ten hours cover a big part of the day and contained
information about how regular the activity was and how active the participant was during
these 10 hours (Burns et al., 2009). Therefore, it was chosen to use the M10 as a variable for
the daily physical activity.
For further research it would be interesting to improve different aspects of the current
study. First of all, it would be interesting to study the relation between daily physical activity

Page 25
and cognition with a bigger sample size, from which more powerful statements could be made
(Neuman, 2014). Secondly, it might be helpful to use other tools to measure daily physical
activity since the M10 does not seem to be the right variable. It would be interesting to
examine if there are more possibilities with the Actiwatch data to split the data into length of
activity, intensity of the activity and day- and night time activity. The more specific
measurements of daily physical activity might be accomplished by a smart watch rather than
an Actiwatch (Xie, 2019). A smart watch could measure length and intensity of the activity,
the time and time of the day in which the activity was accomplished and heart rate of the
participant. Complementary, observation data could be used to see what movements are made
and for how long the activity lasts. Although this is an intensive data collection procedure,
this might give a better insight in the activity pattern of the participant. This insight will lead
to a better view on the type of activity and specific movements the participant makes, which
will give a more controlled representation of the activity pattern of the participant. Besides,
with this information the differences for daily physical activity during daytime and during
night time could be studied. Lastly, another measure for education level could be chosen to
control for the premorbid effects of cognition. According to Legdeur et al. (2017) the Wide
Range Achievement Test - Third Edition (WRAT- III) is a reliable test to measure the
premorbid cognitive functioning for older participants. Also reading level could be a measure
for premorbid cognitive functioning (Miller et al., 2015). This effect is found in different
populations. With the adjustments to the current study as described above, it might be
possible to study the relation between daily physical activity and cognition in a qualitative
higher way.
In conclusion, in this study the relation between daily physical activity and cognition
was examined. Which physical activity interventions have most effect on cognitive
functioning and which patients benefit most from these interventions is still unclear. Despite
the results did not fulfil the expectations, this explorative study could be a good first basis for
further research in which tools could be better used to find the relation between daily physical
activity and cognition in nursing home residents with dementia. This study provides some
handles for new and higher qualitative research which might in the future help to find the
interventions that are effective for specific groups of patients.

Page 26
References
Aalten, P., Van Valen, E., Clare, L., Kenny, G., & Verhey, F. (2005). Awareness in dementia:
A review of clinical correlates. Aging & Mental Health, 9, 414-422. DOI:
10.1080/13607860500143075
Alexopoulos, G. S., Abrams, R. C., Young, R. C., & Shamoian, C. A. (1988). Cornell Scale
for Depression in Dementia. Biological Psychiatry, 23, 271-284.
Barlow, D. H., & Durand, V. M. (2015). Abnormal Psychology: An integrative approach.
Boston: Cengage Learning.
Blondell, S. J., Hammersley-Mather, R., & Veerman, J. L. (2014). Does physical activity
prevent cognitive decline and dementia?: A systematic review and meta-analysis of
longitudinal studies. BMC Public Health, 14, 1-12. DOI: 10.1186/1471-2458-14-510
Burns, A., Allen, H., Tomenson, B., Duignan, D., & Byrne, J. (2009). Bright light therapy for
agitation in dementia: a randomized controlled trial. International Psychogeriatrics,
21, 711-721. DOI: 10.1017/S1041610209008886
Cambridge Neurotechnology. (2008). The Actiwatch User Manual. Cambridge, UK.
Castor EDC. (2019) Developed by Arts, D. in 2011.
Cipriani, G., Lucetti, C., Nuti, A., & Danti, S. (2014). Wandering and dementia.
Psychogeriatrics, 14, 135-142. DOI: 10.1111/psyg.12044
Colcombe, S., & Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness Effects on the Cognitive Function of Older
Adults: A Meta-Analytic Study. Psychological Science, 14, 125-130.
Dröes, R. M. (1993). Cornell scale for depression in dementia. Vakgroep Psychiatrie. Vrije
Universiteit, Amsterdam.
Eggermont, L. H. P., Swaab, D. F., Hol, E. M., & Scherder, E. J. A. (2009). Walking the line:
a randomised trial on the effects of a short term walking programme on cognition in
dementia. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery & psychiatry, 80, 733-736 DOI:
10.1136/jnnp.2008.158444
Eisdorfer, C., Cohen, D., Paveza, G. J., Ashford, J. W., Luchins, D. J., Goreick, P. B.
Hirschman, R. S., Freels, S. A., Levy, P. S., Semla, T. P., & Shaw, H. A. (1993). An
empirical evaluation of the Global Deterioration Scale for staging Alzheimer’s disease.
The American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 681–682. DOI:10.1176/ajp.150.4.681
Engeroff, T., Ingmann, T., & Banzer, W. (2018). Physical Activity Throughout the Adult
Life Span and Domain-Specific Cognitive Function in Old Age: A Systematic Review
of Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Data. Sports Medicine 48, 1405-1436. DOI:
10.1007/s40279-018-0920-6

Page 27
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using
G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research
Methods, 41, 1149-1160.
Folstein, M. F. et al (1975). Journal of Psychiatric Research 12, 189–198.
Fox, N. C., Scahill, R. I., Crum, W. R., & Rossor, M. N. (1999). Correlation between rates of
brain atrophy and cognitive decline in AD. Neurology, 52, 1687-1687. DOI:
10.1212/WNL.52.8.1687
Galea, M., & Woodward, M. (2005). Mini-mental state examination (MMSE). Australian
Journal of Physiotherapy, 51, 198.
Gironda, R. J., Lloyd, J., Clark M. E., & Walker, R. L. (2007). Preliminary evaluation of
reliability and criterion validity of ActiwatchScore. Journal of Rehabilitation Research
& Development, 44, 223, 230.
Gow, A. J., Bastin, M. E., Maniega, S. M., Hernández, M. C. V., Morris, Z., Murray, C.,
Royle, N. A., Starr, J. M., Deary, I. J., & Wardlaw, J. M. (2012). Neuroprotective
lifestyles and the aging brain: activity, atrophy, and white matter integrity.
Neurology, 79, 1802-1808. DOI:10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182703fd2
Groot, C., Hooghiemstra, A. M., Raijmakers, P. G. H. M., van Berckel, B. N. M., Scheltens,
P., Scherder, E. J. A., van der Flier. W. M., & Ossenkoppel, R. (2016). The effect of
physical activity on cognitive function in patients with dementia: A meta-analysis of
randomized control trials. Ageing Research Reviews, 25, 13-23. DOI:
10.1016/j.arr.2015.11.005
Hakala, J. O., Rovio, S. P., Pahkala, K., Nevalainen, J., Juonala, M., Hutri-Kahonen, N.,
Heinonen, O., Hirvensalo, M., Telama, R., Viikari, J. S. A., Tammelin, T. H., &
Raitakari, O. T. (2019). Physical Activity from Childhood to Adulthood and Cognitive
Performance in Midlife. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 51, 882-890.
Hamer, M., & Chida, Y. (2009). Physical activity and risk of neurodegenerative disease: a
systematic review of prospective evidence. Psychological Medicine, 39, 3-11. DOI:
10.1017/S003329170800368
Harris, J. B., & Johnson, C. S. (2017). The Impact of Physical versus Social Activity on the b
Physical and Cognitive Functioning of Seniors with Dementia. Activities, Adaptation
& Aging, 41, 161-174. DOI: 10.1080/01924788.2017.1306383
Henskens M., Nauta I.M., Drost K.T., Milders M.V., & Scherder E.J.A. (2019). Predictors of
care dependency in nursing home residents with moderate to severe dementia: a cross-
sectional study. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 92, 47-54. DOI:

Page 28
10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2018.12.005
Heyn, P. Abreu, B. C. & Ottenbacher, K. J. (2004). The effects of exercise training on elderly
persons with cognitive impairment and dementia: A meta-analysis. Archives of
Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 85, 1694-1704. DOI:
10.1016/j.apmr.2004.03.019
Hillman, C. H., Buck S. M., Themanson J. R., Pontifex M. B., & Castelli D. M. (2009).
Aerobic fitness and cognitive development: event-related brain potential and task
performance indices of executive control in preadolescent children. Developmental
Psychology, 45, 114-29. DOI: 10.1037/a0014437
Hooghiemstra, A. M., Eggermont, L. H.P., Scheltens, P., van der Flier, W. M., & Scherder, E.
J. A. (2015). The Rest-Activity Rhythm and Physical Activity in Early-Onset
Dementia. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders, 29, 45-57. DOI:
10.1097/WAD.0000000000000037
van Hooren, S. A. H., Valentijn, A. M., Bosma, H., Ponds, R. W. H. M., van Boxtel M. P. J.,
& Jolles, J. (2007). Cognitive Functioning in Healthy Older Adults Aged 64–81: A
Cohort Study into the Effects of Age, Sex, and Education. Journal Aging,
Neuropsychology and Cognition, 14, 40-54. DOI: 10.1080/138255890969483
IBM Corp. Released 2017. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0. Armonk, NY:
IBM Corp.
Jack, C. R., Knopman, D. S., Jagust, W. J., Petersen, R. C., Weiner, M. W., Aisen, P. S., ... &
Lesnick, T. G (2013). Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer’s disease:
an updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers. Lancet Neurology, 12, 207-
216. DOI: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70291-0
de Jonghe, J. F. M., Wetzels, R. B., Mulders, A., Zuidema, S. U., & Koopmans, R. T. (2009).
Validity of the severe impairment battery short version. Journal of Neurology,
Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 80, 954-959.
Kikkert, L. H. J., Vuillerme N., van Campend, J. P., Hortobágyi, T., & Lamotha, C. J. (2016).
Walking ability to predict future cognitive decline in old adults: A scoping review.
Ageing Research Reviews, 27, 1-14. DOI: 10.1016/j.arr.2016.02.001
Koenig, A. M., DeLozier, I. J., Zmuda, M. D., Marron, M. M., Begley, A. E., Anderson, S. J.,
... & Butters, M. A. (2015). Neuropsychological functioning in the acute and remitted
states of late- life depression. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 45,175-185.
Kuring, J. K., Mathias, J. L., & Ward, L. (2018). Prevalence of Depression, Anxiety and
PTSD in People with Dementia: a Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis.

Page 29
Neuropsychology Review, 28, 393-416. DOI: 10.1007/s11065-018-9396-2
Legdeur, N., Binnekade, T. T., Otten, R. H., Badissia, M., Scheltens, P., Visser, P. J., &
Maiere, A. B. (2017). Cognitive functioning of individuals aged 90 years and older
without dementia: A systematic review. Ageing Research Reviews, 36, 42-49.
DOI:10.1016/j.arr.2017.02.006
Leisman, G. (2011). Brain networks, plasticity, and functional connectivities inform current
directions in functional neurology and rehabilitation. Funct Neurol Rehab Ergon, 1,
315-56.
Leisman, G., Moustafa, A. A., & Shafir, T. (2016). Thinking, Walking, Talking: Integratory
Motor and Cognitive Brain Function. Frontiers in public health, 4, 94.
Leng, Y., Musiek, E. S., Hu, K., Cappuccio, F. P., & Yaffe, K. (2019). Association between
circadian rhythms and neurodegenerative diseases. The Lancet Neurology, 18, 307-
318. DOI: 10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30461-7
Litchke, L. G., Hodges, J. S., & Reardon, R. F. (2012). Benefits of chair yoga for persons with
mild to severe Alzheimer’s disease. Activities, Adaptation & Aging, 36, 317–328.
DOI:10.1080/01924788.2012.729185
Lucas, S. J. E., Ainslie, P. N., Murrell, C. J., Thomas, K. N., Franz, E. A., Cotter, J. D. (2012).
Effect of age on exercise-induced alterations in cognitive executive function:
Relationship to cerebral perfusion. Experimental Gerontology, 47, 541-551. DOI:
10.1016/j.exger.2011.12.002
McDonnell, M. N., Buckley J. D., Opie, G. M., Ridding, M. C., & Semmler, J. G. (2013). A
single bout of aerobic exercise promotes motor cortical neuroplasticity. Journal of
Applied Physiology 114, 1174-82. DOI:10.1152/japplphysiol.01378.2012
Miller, I. N., Himali, J. J., Beiser, A. S., Murabito, J. M., Seshadri, S., Wolf, P. A., & Au, R.
(2015). Normative Data for the Cognitively Intact Oldest-Old: The Framingham Heart
Study. Experimental Aging Research, 41, 386-409. DOI:
10.1080/0361073X.2015.1053755
Miu, D. K. Y., Szeto, S. L., & Mak,Y. F. (2008). A randomized controlled trial on the effect
of exercise on physical, cognitive and affective function in dementia subjects. Asian
J Gerontol Geriatr, 3, 8-16.
Moore, S., Sandman, C. A., McGrady, K., & Kesslak, J. P. (2010). Memory training improves
cognitive ability in patients with dementia. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 11,
245-261.
Mourao, R. J., Mansur, G., Malloy-Diniz, L. F., Costa, E. C., & Diniz, B. S. (2015).

Page 30
Depressive symptoms increase the risk of progression to dementia in subjects with
mild cognitive impairment: systematic review and meta‐analysis. International
journal of Geriatric psychiatry, 31, 905-911. DOI: 10.1002/gps.4406
Neuman, W. L. (2014). Understanding Research. London, United Kingdom: Pearson.
O’Connor. D. W., Pollitt, P. A., Hyde, J. B., Fellows, J. L., Miller, N. D., Brook, C. P. B., &
Reiss, B. B. (1889). The Reliability and validity of the Mini-Mental State Examination
in a British community survey. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 23, 87-96.
Park, J., & Cohen, I. (2019). Effects of Exercise Interventions in Older Adults with Various
Types of Dementia: Systematic Review. Activities, Adaptation & Aging, 43, 87-117.
DOI: 10.1080/01924788.2018.1493897
Perea, R. D., Vidoni, E. D., Morris, J. K., Graves, R. S., Burns, J. M., & Honea, R. A. (2016).
Cardiorespiratory fitness and white matter integrity in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain
Imaging and Behavior, 10, 60–66. DOI:10.1007/s11682-015-9431-3
Prakash, R. S., Voss, M. W., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2015). Physical Activity and
Cognitive Vitality. Annual Reviews Psychology, 66, 769-97.
Qazi, A., Richardson, B., Simmons, P., Mullan, E., Walker, Z., Katona, C., & Orrell, M.
(2005). The Mini‐SIB: a short scale for measuring cognitive function in severe
dementia. International journal of geriatric psychiatry, 20, 1001-1002.
Reas, E. T., Laughlin, G. A., Bergstrom, J., Kritz-Silverstein, D., Richard, E. L., Barrett-
Connor, E., & Mcevoy, L. K. (2019). Lifetime physical activity and late-life cognitive
function: the Rancho Bernardo study. Age and Ageing, 48, 241-246. DOI:
10.1093/ageing/afy188
Reisberg, B., Ferris, S. H., de Leon, M. J., & Crook, T. (1982). The Global Deterioration
Scale for assessment of primary degenerative dementia. The American journal of
psychiatry, 139, 1136-1139.
Sofi, F., Capalbo, A., Marcucci, R., Gori, A. M. Fedi, S., Macchi, C., Casini, A., Surrenti, C.,
Abbate, R., & Gensini, G. F. (2007). Leisure time but not occupational physical
activity significantly affects cardiovascular risk factors in an adult population.
European Journal of Clinical Investigation, 37, 947-953. DOI: 10.1111/j.1365-v
2362.2007.01884.x
Sofi, F., Valecchi, D., Bacci, D., Abbate, R., Gensini, G. F., Casini, A.& Macchi, C. (2010).
Physical activity and risk of cognitive decline: a meta-analysis of prospective studies.
Journal of Internal Medicine, 269, 107-117. DOI:10.1111/j.1365-2796.2010.02281.x
Stern, Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurology, 11,

Page 31
1006-12.
Tappen, R. M., Roach, K. E., Buchner, D., Barry, C., & Edelstein, J. (1997). Reliability of
Physical Performance Measures in Nursing Home Residents With Alzheimer's
Disease. The Journals of Gerontology, 52, 52-55.
Varela, S., Ayán, C., Cancela, J. M., & Martín, V. (2011). Effects of two different intensities
of aerobic exercise on elderly people with mild cognitive impairment: a randomized
pilot study. Clinical Rehabilitation, 26, 442–450. DOI: 10.1177/0269215511425835
Verhage, F. (1964). Intelligentie en leeftijd bij volwassenen en bejaarden. Groningen:
Koninklijke Van Gorcum.
Vinkers, D. J., Gussekloo, J., Stek, M. L., Westendorp, R. G. J., & van der Mast R. C. (2004).
Temporal Relation Between Depression And Cognitive Impairment In Old Age:
Prospective Population Based Study. British Medical Journal, 329, 881-883. DOI:
10.1136/bmj.38216.604664
Voelcker-Rehage, C., & Niemann, C. (2013). Structural and functional brain changes related
to different types of physical activity across the life span. Neuroscience Biobehavioral
Review 37, 2268-2295. DOI:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.028
van der Waerden, B.L. (1952). Order tests for the two-sample problem and their power.
Indagationes Mathematicae, 14, 453–458.
Wajman, J., R., & Bertolucci, P., H., F. (2006). Comparison between neuropsychological
evaluation instruments for severe dementia. Arquivos de neuro-psiquiatria, 64, 736-
740.
Xie, Z. (2019). Using Smartwatch and Bluetooth Beacons to Monitor Physical Activity of
Older Adults (Doctoral dissertation, UCLA).
Yágüez, L., Shaw, K. N., Morris, R., & Matthews, D. (2011). The effects on cognitive
functions of a movement‐based intervention in patients with Alzheimer's type
dementia: a pilot study. International journal of Geriatric psychiatry, 26, 173-181.
DOI: 10.1002/gps.2510
Zarit, S. H., Zarit, J. M., & Reever, K. E. (1982). Memory Training for Severe Memory Loss:
Effects on Senile Dementia Patients and Their Families. The Gerontologist, 22, 373-
377. DOI: 10.1093/geront/22.4.373